220 research outputs found

    Postretrieval overconfidence and anosognosia in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD)

    Full text link
    General self awareness (anosognosia) and metacognitive monitoring in memory tasks are both impaired in AD, but how they relate to each other is still an open question. We examined awareness with the Anosognosia Questionnaire Dementia (AQD) and monitoring within a memory task, during retrieval with feeling-of-knowing (FOK) and post-retrieval, with judgment-of-confidence (JOC). FOKs/JOCs were performed for names of people either previously linked to self or other. AD showed both impaired FOK and JOC. They also showed lower self metamemory effect in their JOCs and lower awareness of their behavioral functioning in the AQD, which was specifically related to overconfidence in their JOCs for self-related items. Thus, anosognosia and altered postretrieval monitoring for self-related information may be related in AD

    Imaging-based parcellations of the human brain

    Full text link
    A defining aspect of brain organization is its spatial heterogeneity, which gives rise to multiple topographies at different scales. Brain parcellation — defining distinct partitions in the brain, be they areas or networks that comprise multiple discontinuous but closely interacting regions — is thus fundamental for understanding brain organization and function. The past decade has seen an explosion of in vivo MRI-based approaches to identify and parcellate the brain on the basis of a wealth of different features, ranging from local properties of brain tissue to long-range connectivity patterns, in addition to structural and functional markers. Given the high diversity of these various approaches, assessing the convergence and divergence among these ensuing maps is a challenge. Inter-individual variability adds to this challenge but also provides new opportunities when coupled with cross-species and developmental parcellation studies

    Towards increasing the clinical applicability of machine learning biomarkers in psychiatry.

    Full text link
    Due to a lack of objective biomarkers, psychiatric diagnoses still rely strongly on patient reporting and clinician judgement. The ensuing subjectivity negatively affects the definition and reliability of psychiatric diagnoses1,2. Recent research has suggested that a combination of advanced neuroimaging and machine learning may provide a solution to this predicament by establishing such objective biomarkers for psychiatric conditions, improving the diagnostic accuracy, prognosis and development of novel treatments3.These promises led to widespread interest in machine learning applications for mental health4, including a recent paper that reports a biological marker for one of the most difficult yet momentous questions in psychiatry—the assessment of suicidal behaviour5. Just et al. compared a group of 17 participants with suicidal ideation with 17 healthy controls, reporting high discrimination accuracy using task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging signatures of life- and death-related concepts3. The authors further reported high discrimination between nine ideators who had attempted suicide versus eight ideators who had not. While being a laudable effort into a difficult topic, this study unfortunately illustrates some common conceptual and technical issues in the field that limit translation into clinical practice and raise unrealistic hopes when the results are communicated to the general public.From a conceptual point of view, machine learning studies aimed at clinical applications need to carefully consider any decisions that might hamper the interpretation or generalizability of their results. Restrictiveness to an arbitrary setting may become detrimental for machine learning applications by providing overly optimistic results that are unlikely to generalize. As an example, Just et al. excluded more than half of the patients and healthy controls initially enrolled in the study from the main analysis due to missing desired functional magnetic resonance imaging effects (a rank accuracy of at least 0.6 based on all 30 concepts). This exclusion introduces a non-assessable bias to the interpretation of the results, in particular when considering that only six of the 30 concepts were selected for the final classification procedure. While Just et al. attempt to address this question by applying the trained classifier to the initially excluded 21 suicidal ideators, they explicitly omit the excluded 24 controls from this analysis, preventing any interpretation of the extent to which the classifier decision is dependent on this initial choice.From a technical point of view, machine learning-based predictions based on neuroimaging data in small samples are intrinsically highly variable, as stable accuracy estimates and high generalizability are only achieved with several hundreds of participants6,7. The study by Just et al. falls into this category of studies with a small sample size. To estimate the impact of uncertainty on the results by Just et al., we adapted a simulation approach with the code and data kindly provided by the authors, randomly permuting (800 times) the labels across the groups using their default settings and computing the accuracies. These results showed that the 95% confidence interval for classification accuracy obtained using this dataset is about 20%, leaving large uncertainty with respect to any potential findings.Special care is also required with respect to any subjective choices in feature and classifier settings or group selection. While ad-hoc selection of a specific setting is subjective, testing of different ones and outcome-based post-hoc justification of such leads to overfitting, thus limiting the generalizability of any classification. Such overfitting may occur when multiple models or parameter choices are tested with respect to their ability to predict the testing data and only those that perform best are reported. To illustrate this issue, we performed an additional analysis with the code and data kindly provided by Just et al. More specifically, in the code and the manuscript, we identified the following non-exhaustive number of prespecified settings: (1) removal of occipital cortex data; (2) subdivision of clusters larger than 11 mm; (3) selection of voxels with at least four contributing participants in each group; (4) selection of stable clusters containing at least five voxels; (5) selection of the 1,200 most stable features; and (6) manual copying and replacing of a cluster for one control participant. Importantly, according to the publication or code documentation, all of these parameters were chosen ad hoc and for none of these settings was a parameter search performed. We systematically evaluated the effect of each of these choices on the accuracy for differentiation between suicide ideators and controls in the original dataset provided by Just et al. As shown in Fig. 1, each of the six parameters represents an optimum choice for differentiation accuracy in this dataset, with any (even minor) change often resulting in substantially lower accuracy estimates. Similarly, data leakage may also contribute to optimistic results when information outside the training set is used to build a prediction model. More generally, whenever human interventions guide the development of machine learning models for the prediction of clinical conditions, a careful evaluation and reporting of any researcher’s degrees of freedom is essential to avoid data leakage and overfitting. Subsequent sharing of data processing and analysis pipelines, as well as collected data, is a further key step to increase reproducibility and facilitate replication of potential findings

    Modulating effect of COMT genotype on the brain regions underlying proactive control process during inhibition

    Full text link
    peer reviewedIntroduction. Genetic variability related to the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene (Val158Met polymorphism) has received increasing attention as a possible modulator of cognitive control functions. Methods. In an event-related fMRI study, a modified version of the Stroop task was administered to three groups of 15 young adults according to their COMT Val158Met genotype [Val/Val (VV), Val/Met (VM) and Met/Met (MM)]. Based on the theory of dual mechanisms of control (Braver, et al., 2007), the Stroop task has been built to induce proactive or reactive control processes according to the task context. Results. Behavioral results did not show any significant group differences for reaction times but Val allele carriers individuals are less accurate in the processing of incongruent items. fMRI results revealed that proactive control is specifically associated with increased activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in carriers of the Met allele, while increased activity is observed in the middle frontal gyrus (MFG) in carriers of the Val allele. Conclusion. These observations, in keeping with a higher cortical dopamine level in MM individuals, support the hypothesis of a COMT Val158Met genotype modulation of the brain regions underlying proactive control, especially in frontal areas as suggested by Braver et al
    • …
    corecore